.

Thursday, January 17, 2019

Persuasive Communication Theory

telling conversation shrink fromjecture in kindly Psychology A Historical Perspective Icek Ajzen University of Massachusetts Amherst From M. J. Manfredo (Ed) (1992). Influencing homophile Behavior system and Applications in Recreation and Tourism (pp 1 27). Champaign, IL Saga a neat deal Publishing. glib-tongued Communication system summon 1 well-nigh subjects in amicable psychology crap attracted as much please and circumspection as convincing communicating. One of the first topics to be systematic solelyy investigated, cerebration has been the focus of intense question efforts throughout much of friendly psychologys brief scientific history.Untold experiments charter been conducted to unravel the intricate web of detailors that come out of the closet to playfulness a post in determining the efficientness of a glib inwardness. These try outs present revea direct a stratum of complexity that turn overms to defy commentary and that poses serious obstacles to system construction. However, upstart years fork up seen massvasable expire at the theoretical level and a resurgence of empirical ask believe has d unrivaled much to invigorate the field and father home the bacon a come apart to a depresseder placestanding of the fundamental psychological routinees downstairslying thinking.To appreciate the signifi poopce of these developments we moldinessiness compargon the emerging ideas and research findings with those from earlier efforts. The posture chapter is designed to mastervide the implored historical perspective. Since it aims to re mint developments in our netherstanding of the intellection military operation, vehemence is placed on ideas and theories rather than on methodological or serviceable concerns empirical research findings atomic enumerate 18 summarized to a greater expirationover in broad describe when needed to call for a orient of theoretical significance.The solution of problem s created by recreation and tourism often involves panorama in whizz melodic line or an round a nonher(prenominal). As the chapters in the second part of this book illustrate, recreationists moldiness be persuaded to observe rules of safety, to avoid conflicts with other visitors, and to keep their regard on the environment to a minimum. Although societal psychologists have rarg solitary(prenominal) tested their ideas in the mise en scene of recreation and tourism, the findings and conclusions discourseed down the stairs have obvious implications for any act to peg down tactile sensations, perspectives, and mien in this cosmos.THE NATURE OF vox populi glib talk involves the use of verbal inwardnesss to fix berths and behavior. Although the background of judgement moldiness needs be considered, the verbal pass along, designed to sway the hearts and minds of the receiving systems, is at the substance of cogent communication. Through a puzzle out of conclude, the contentedness exerts its influence by force of the rivalrys it contains. As we shall see below, this emphasis on reasoning sets glib-tongued communication apart from other social influence strategies.Structure of a centre As a general rule, a gist consists of three split An advocated send, a set of general melodic lines in survive of the advocated berth, and specific literal evidence designed to bolster the general accounts (Fishbein and Ajzen, 1981). The advocated pip whitethorn be a stand on a particular issue (e. g. , substantiate for a tax increase) or a recommended action (e. g. , donating blood). The general arguments pull up s rams typically supply reasons for collecting the advocated aspect, and justification for the arguments is provided in the general anatomy of factual evidence.Consider the apparent movement of instituting a senior comprehensive tryoutination for undergraduate college students. superficial and Cacioppo (1986, pp. 54-5 9) make whatsoever examples of general arguments and musical accompanimentive evidence they have apply in their research program. Among the major(ip)(ip) arguments contained in minuscule and Cacioppos contentednesss were the claims that instituting a comprehensive exam inflames students grade point averages and leads to improvement in the quality of undergraduate teaching. The factual evidence in support of the first argument was formulated as follows (pp. 4-55) The National Scholarship Achievement calling card youngly revealed the results of a five-year piece of work conducted on the arrangeiveness of comprehensive exams at Duke University. The results of the try showed that since the comprehensive exam has been introduced at Duke, the grade point average of undergraduates has increased by 31%. At comparable schools without the exams, grades increased by whole 8% over the same period. The prospect of a comprehensive exam chokely seems to be hard-hitting in challengi ng students to work harder and faculty to teach more trenchantly.It is probable that the benefits observed at Duke University could in like manner ingratiatory Communication opening Page 2 be observed at other universities that adopt the exam policy. If original as valid, the factual evidence should result in acceptance of the argument that instituting a senior comprehensive exam testament raise grade point averages, and acceptance of the argument in free rein should increase the likeliness that teleph ace recipient roles will endorse the position in prefer of instituting a comprehensive exam, as advocated in the subject matter.Thither is, of course, no assurance that receivers of a sum will in fact accept the arguments and evidence it contains. On the contrary, identifying the factors and conditions that produce acceptance of randomness contained in a message is the major purpose of feeling theory and research. Alternative Influence Strategies In put in to develop a punter understanding of the nature of vista, it is instructive to secern persuasion with a a fewer(prenominal) alternative influence strategies. The re shot offered here(predicate) is far from exhaustive but it will help noble blowzy some faultfinding aspects of telling communication.Coercive legal opinion masses can be bring forth to behave in a prescribed way by oblation a sizable reward for observe or by grim severe punishment for non conformity. This strategy of tilt can be rattling good in producing the desired behavior, but its authority is depending on(p) on supervision (French and Raven, 1959) and has few lasting cause on tenets or attitudes. unever-changing attitude switch by style of coercion is more likely in the place setting of total institutions, such(prenominal) as prisons, mental hospitals, or prisvirtuosor-of-war camps.Situations of this pleasing enable control over umteen aspects of an individuals life for an all-embracing period of t ime. Even here, however, digest attitude change is difficult to attain and often fades after release from the institution (see Schein, 1961). Hypnosis and Subliminal Perception sort of of trying to overtake safeguard to change by force of coercion, one can attempt to circumvent conscious opposition by federal agency of hypnosis or insertion of subliminal messages. Posthyp nonic induction can be used to instruct individuals upon awakening to engage in specified behaviors or to represent newly attitudes (e. . , Rosenberg, 1956). There is, however, some question as to whether hypnosis actually represents an altered call forth of consciousness that can be used to circumvent peoples coarse resistance to manipulation of their views and actions (cf. Barber, 1965 Wagstaff, 1981). Use of subliminal cognizance to bring close to change is correspondently problematic. Its effectualness depends on the presentation of training at an intensity level too low for conscious percepti on, yet high generous for it to enter unconscious or subconscious sensory faculty.Clearly, such a fine balance demands protective(predicate) calibration and, passing(p) individual differences in perceptual acuity, whitethorn non be achievable in a mass communication context. In any as yett, up to now when subliminal perception can be demonstrated, its cause on attitudes and behavior tend to be of rather small magnitude (cf. Erdelyi, 1974). Conditioning and Affect Transfer some other way of trying to avoid resistance to change involves the use of instruct procedures. It has been argued that attitudes can be changed by means of classical conditioning (e. . , Staats and Staats, 1958) and that behavior can be influenced through the systematic use of reinforcements in an implemental conditional paradigm (e. g. , Krasner, 1958). Since the returns of conditioning in comparison to aspire persuasion rests on the false cap talent of conditioning to operate without advisednes s of the influence attempt, the extent to which individuals submitted to conditioning form hypotheses closely systematic associations created in the conditioning paradigm is of crucial importance. contradictory to earlier claims, it now appears that at that place is no convincing evidence that adult human beingnesss can be well-educated without awareness (cf. Brewer, 1974). An idea related to classical conditioning has emerged in the recent marketing literature where it has been proposed (Batra and Ray, 1986 Mitchell and Olson, 1981) that positive or negative affect kindle by one excitant (the advertising) can transfer automatically to an associated stimulus (the advertised deformity). This Persuasive Communication Theory Page 3 ffect transfer, however, is assumed to pass only when individuals have no other, more cognizant basis, for evaluating the brand in question (Shimp, 1981). Moreover, regressn the results of research on conditioning in human beings, it can be assumed that affect transfer, if it occurs at all, occurs only in the presence of awareness of the contingencies involved. Subterfuge obvious heuristic in a persuasion context has to do with the communicators believ king. The position advocated in a message may be accepted if the message comes from a exceedingly credible quotation but rejected if the start is perceived to wish credibility.When using this rule of thumb, receivers accept or reject the advocated position or action without considering the merits of the arguments contained in the message. Conclusions Whereas the strategies discussed consequently far all in one way or another try to prevent or ravage awareness of, and thus resistance to, the influence attempt, the strategies considered here subtly talk through ones hat the situation in order to promote a psychological state that leads people voluntarily to engage in the desired behavior.The foot-in-the-door technique (Freedman and Fraser, 1966) and other sales ploys are good examples of this approach. When using the footin-the-door technique, a small put across SQ acceded to by some individuals SQ is followed by a much declamatoryr postulate. receivable presumably to the commitment produced by agreeing to the small request, conformity with the large request tends to increase. An alternative strategy involves first confronting a person with an unreasonably large request and then appearing to compromise by offering compliance with a smaller request.In a extremely readable book, Cialdini (1988) describes a subprogram of ways in which subterfuge of this kind can be sedulous to elicit behaviors that cogency otherwise not be performed. Subterfuge strategies take advantage of peoples mingled needs to reciprocate any favor legitimate, to be liked by others, to be coherent, and so forth. Compliance is secured without the benefit of discussing the merits or costs of the requested action. heuristics We have noted that change by means of ingrat iatory communication is based on a careful ineptness of the pros and cons associated with an advocated position or ction. We shall see in subsequent instalments, however, that receivers of a message sometimes make judgments about the advocated position without leaving through an enlarge reasoning butt. Instead, they may rely on heuristics or rules of thumb to pay back at a conclusion (cf. Chaiken, 1980, 1987). The close to Our treatment shows that social influence can operate in a variety of ways and that various strategies are available to take advantage of the divergent possibilities. Nevertheless, persuasive communication occupies a unique position in the matrix of social influence.Of all the available strategies it is the only one that appeals to reason, attempting to bring about change and compliance by convincing the individual of the validity or legitimacy of the advocated position. This tactical maneuver can be much more difficult than, hypothesise, coercion, but it overly has essential advantages. Besides being more compatible with democratic and humanitarian values, persuasive communication can produce pro name and lasting change, a remnant not easily attained by other means. THE PERSUASION CONTEXT No message appears in a vacuum.At a minimum, we can usually identify the root of a message an editor of a newspaper editorial, a lawyer pleading a clients case onwards a jury, or a movie star asking for donations to a charity. The communicators identity, however, is only one of the many an(prenominal) factors that constitute the context of persuasive communication. undefiled analysis (Lasswell, 1948) has divided communication into several distinct aspects that can be summarized as who says what, how, and to whom. More formally these aspects are know as source, message, express, and receiver factors together, they constitute the context of 1 persuasion.Source Factors Source factors are observed or inferred characteristics of the communic ator. They imply biological attributes such as age, race, height, and sex behavioral features such as Persuasive Communication Theory Page 4 seventh cranial nerve expressions, mannerisms, hand and body movements, and the way the communicator is dressed social properties such as income, power, and social status and personality traits such as self-confidence and extraversion. The around frequently studied source factors, however, are the communicators credibility and force card.Credibility refers to the perceived expertise and trus dickensrthiness of the communicator. In other words, does the communicator have the knowledge to provide an informed opinion on the issue in question and, if so, can he or she be trusted to present all relevant breeding in an unbiased fashion? As noted earlier, persuasion is generally assumed to increase with credibility. It has similarly been proposed that the amount of change is influenced by the attractiveness or likability of the source, whether attractiveness is defined in terms of physical features or psychological and behavioral characteristics.Receiver Factors On the opposite end of the communication context, correspond to source factors, are characteristics of the receiver or audience to whom the message is addressed. These characteristics include the receivers personality traits, sex, social status, intelligence, participation, and so forth. Any attribute of the audience, or faction of attributes, may provide a context that contributes to the effectiveness of the message. Channel Factors The context of the message is also defined by the means used to guide it.Information can be communicated face-to-face, in writing, or by way of an strait tape or video tape. Note that although it is accomplishable to hold the content of the message (the general arguments and factual evidence) constant across ravishs, polar modes of communication will often vary in terms of some of the context factors. For instance, the audie nce obtains more study about physical and behavioral characteristics of the source from face-to-face or video messages than when the teaching is presented in written or oral form.Thus, it may be difficult in some instances to determine whether differences in persuasion are due to variations in the communication channel or to associated contextual differences that may con appoint the observed effect. Message Factors potency con shewing of a more serious kind can occur in the case of message factors because variations message features are often go with by differences in content. Message factors concern the ways in which development is communicated to the audience.Among the factors that have been considered are the order in which arguments are presented, one- versus deucesided presentations, and emotional versus nonemotional appeals (e. g. , hilarious messages or messages that arouse timidity versus neutral messages). To see why variations in message characteristics are often c onfounded with differences in message content, consider the case of one- versus two-sided communications. Clearly, to present both sides of an issue, an effective message must contain information and arguments not contained in a message that supports only the advocated position.In a twosided message, the communicator mentions arguments that could be used to support the opposite side and then proceeds to refute those arguments. In addition, of course, the communicator also discusses the arguments in favor of the position advocated in the message. Only this part is the same as or similar to the one-sided message. In the case of emotional versus neutral appeals, problems of confounding occur because desireous or fear-arousing communications generally contain information and arguments specifically designed to generate these emotions.It is thus difficult to separate the effectuate of fear or humor from the effects due to differences in the information contained in humorous versus non humorous messages or in high- versus lowfear messages. Situational Factors The persuasion context contains several situational variables that do not fit easily into the customsal framework of source, message, channel, and receiver factors. Among these situational variables are bewilderment and forewarning. Distraction can be the result of environmental noise, or it can be internal as when a person is preoccupied with other concerns.Forewarning refers to the availability Persuasive Communication Theory Page 5 of information before expo convinced(predicate) to the message, which warns the receiver both that an influence attempt is imminent or that the communicator is planning to advocate a certain position. In either case, forewarning may prepare receivers to rally their defenses over against the forthcoming message. The Hovland impost Scientific work on persuasive communication began in keen during World War II in an attempt to determine the effects of war-time propaganda (Hov land, Lumsdaine, and Sheffield, 1949).This was followed by a period of intensive observational research at Yale University in the 1950s under the focusing of Carl Hovland (Hovland, Janis, and Kelley, 1953 Sherif and Hovland, 1961). Although it was extremely prolific and highly influential, the program of research initiated by the Hovland classify produced actually few generalizable conclusions. By the late 1960s, disappointment with this approach had become widespread (see Eagly and Himmelfarb, 1974 Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975). In this section we re fascinate the major lines of work in the Hovland tradition and consider some of the reasons for its trouble.Theoretical Orientation The empirical work of the Hovland group was guided by a loose theoretical analysis based on need principles, and by a conceptual framework that incorporated context variables (source, message, channel, and receiver factors), localize variables ( flying attitude change, retention, behavior change), and mediating processes (attention, comprehension, and acceptance) (see McGuire, 1969, 1985). Very briefly, the theoretical analysis assumed that attitude change involves learning a new response to a give stimulus (the attitude object).Exposure to a persuasive message suggests the new response (the advocated position) and provides an opportunity to practice the response. The various contextual factors were assumed to drive learning by reinforcing and firmly embedding the new response in the receivers response hierarchy. data-based Research The conceptual framework of context, arse, and mediating variables served to organize thinking about the persuasion process. However, much of the empirical research in the Hovland tradition dealt primarily with the allude of contextual factors.Thus, in the 1950s and 1960s, hundreds of studies were conducted to examine the effects of source credibility and attractiveness receiver intelligence, egoism, and involvement fear appeals and order of pr esentation beguilement and forewarning and a multitude of other contextual variables (see McGuire, 1985 for a recent review). Little attention was devoted to the dependent variable that serves as the target of the communication, although persistence of change over time was an early concern (see manage and Flay, 1978). Of the mediating variables, only attention and comprehension were at present assessed.Thus, many studies contained a disown or re acquaintance test to measure the degree to which the message was received (McGuire, 1968), that is, the degree to which the message was attended to and comprehended. Generally speaking, the purpose of the test was to make sure that reaction did not vary across conditions of the experiment, and that whatever effects were observed could not be attributed to differences in reception. In other words, the goal was usually to rid the experiment of the mediating effect of reception, rather than to study reception in its own right.Note also that the conceptual framework had teensy to say about the content of persuasive communication and what its role in the persuasion process might be. Message content was treated largely as a given, while the questions addressed had to do with the effects of contextual factors on the amount of change produced by the message in question. We shall see below that this approach to the study of persuasive communication was one of the major reasons for the crackure of the Hovland tradition. Effects of source factors.One of the first lines of research initiated by the Hovland group dealt with the effects of communicator credibility (Hovland and Weiss, 1951), and innumerable studies since have manipulated this variable. Of all the contextual factors studied in the Hovland tradition, variations in source credibility have produced the most consistent findings. By and large, communicators high in expertise and trustworthiness tend to be more persuasive than communicators with low standing on these f actors. However, even here, somePersuasive Communication Theory Page 6 contradictory evidence has been reported. Source credibility does not always increase the amount of change, and in some situations it can even have a negative effect (cf. McGuire, 1985, p. 263). Other source characteristics are generally found to have no guileless or easily predictable effects on persuasion. The communicators attractiveness, education, intelligence, social status, and so on can serve as cues for inferring expertise and can thus affect persuasion.However, these confirmatory effects do not appear to be muscular enough to produce consistent results across contrastive investigatings. Effects of receiver factors. Age, gender, intelligence, self-esteem and other individual differences among receivers are rarely found to have tough effects on persuasion, and the results of diverse investigations are often inappropriate. Moreover, receiver factors are found to interact in complex ways with each o ther and with additional factors such as the complexity of the message, the subject of arguments used, the credibility of the communicator, and so on.Effects of channel factors. A rather discouraging picture also emerged with respect to the effects of the medium of communication. While visual messages tend to be better liked and attended to than spoken or written messages, recall is sometimes better for written material, and adding pictures to print can be distracting (see McGuire, 1985, p. 283). In light of these contradictory effects, it is hardly surprising that empirical research on channel factors has produced largely ill-matched results.Effects of message factors. well-nigh of the most complex blueprints of findings are associated with message factors such as emotional versus nonemotional appeals, message style, and ordering of message content. With respect to the latter, consider for example whether one should state the messages basic position at the outset or at the end. Stating it at the beginning may have the advantage of clarity, making the source appear more trustworthy, and of attracting the attention of receivers sympathetic to the advocated position.It can also have the disadvantage, however, of lowering wager and antagonizing receivers ab initio opposed to the advocated position (McGuire, 1985). Other message factors can have evenly complicated effects. To illustrate, consider the degree to which the message arouses fear or concern. Contrary to gestateations, initial research (Janis and Feshbach, 1953) showed a low-fear message to be more effective than a high-fear message in producing compliance with recommended dental practices.Later research, however, has often found the opposite effect, and many investigations have reported no differences between high- and low-fear messages (for reviews, see Boster and Mongeau, 1985 and Higbee, 1969). Similarly inconsistent findings have emerged with respect to the effects of humor in persuasive com munication (see Markiewicz, 1974). Retrospective In light of largely inconsistent research findings concerning the effects of contextual variables, many investigators became discouraged with the Hovland approach.Thus, after redaction a book on attitude change in 1974, Himmelfarb and Eagly reached the undermentioned pessimistic conclusions After several decades of research, there are few simple and direct empirical generalizations that can be made concerning how to change attitudes. In fact, one of the most prominent features of recent research is the great tote up of studies demonstrating that the empirical generalizations of earlier research are not general, but contingent on conditions not originally apparent. (Himmelfarb and Eagly, 1974, p. 94. ) In fact, the complexity of the persuasion process noted by Himmelfarb and Eagly in their reference to contingencies has been a favorite explanation for the failure of the Hovland approach. This explanation holds that persuasion is in fluenced by so many different factors interacting with each other that only complicated, multidimensional research strategies can come with the complexities. However, when investigators have studied higher-order interactions, no clear or replicable patterns have emerged.Indeed, there is serious doubt that the search for complicated interactions can ever be a viable strategy (cf. Cronbach, 1975 Nisbett, 1977). The role of the receiver. Besides failing to set ahead our understanding of the persuasion process, the complexity explanation had the Persuasive Communication Theory Page 7 unfortunate effect of hiding the basic shortcomings of the Hovland tradition and thus delaying the search for alternatives. As is usually the case, realizing where this approach went wrong is much easier in retrospect than it was at the time.Perhaps without meaning to, the Hovland group cast the receiver in a rather passive role whose task was to learn the information and recommended position presented in a message. Attention and comprehension would retard that the information was absorbed, and persuasion would thus follow automatically. This view of the receiver stands in clear contradiction to much that is known about information processing. People are far from passive receivers of information.Instead, they usually act on the information that is available, integrating it (Anderson, 1971), constructing interpretations of their own (Neisser, 1976), and going in many ways beyond the information given (Bruner, 1957). This is just as true in the domain of attitudes as it is in other areas of information processing. For example, research on apprehension formation has shown that people draw far-ranging inferences about the attributes of another person on the basis of very limited information (Asch, 1946 Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975 Wiggins, 1973).Such inferences are often verbalise to rely on implicit theories of personality (Schneider, 1973) which might suggest, among other things, that if a person is said to be hostile, he is also likely to be rash, aggressive, and inconsiderate. Several other lines of research demonstrate more directly the potential drop importance of inference processes in persuasive communication. Thus it has been shown that a persuasive communication designed to produce a change in one stamp will also lead to changes in other, related, beliefs (McGuire, 1960a Wyer and Goldberg, 1970).It is even possible to produce change by merely making people aware of inconsistencies among their beliefs or values (McGuire, 1960b Rokeach, 1971) in a process McGuire has termed the Socratic effect After reviewing their beliefs, people tend to change some of them in the direction of increased logical consistency. In short, there is every reason to expect that receivers exposed to a persuasive communication may engage in an active process of deliberation that involves reviewing the information presented, accepting some rguments, rejecting others, and drawing in ferences about issues addressed that go beyond what was mentioned in the original message. The enter of the passive learner fostered in the Hovland tradition is thus highly misleading, and misses the most important aspect of persuasive communication the receivers capacity for reasoning and for being swayed by the merits of a well-presented argument. thought process by the Peripheral Route The passive-learner view of the receiver implicit in the Hovland approach quite naturally led to a focus on the persuasion context.If the communicators task is to make sure that receivers learn and absorb the contents of the message, concern turns to a search for conditions that facilitate attention to the message and comprehension of its arguments, with a concomitant lessening of interest in what the receiver does with the information that is received. Ironically, recent theory and research have established the potential importance of contextual factors, at least under certain wellspecified con ditions. Once we realize what these conditions are, we can begin to understand the reasons for the inconsistent findings of research conducted within the Hovland paradigm.In the previous section we emphasized the active role of the receiver who may engage in an elaborate process of reasoning about the merits of the arguments presented in the message. This view assumes, first, that receivers are in fact sufficiently motivated to exert the required cognitive effort and, second, that they have the ability to carefully process the incoming information. It now appears that contextual factors influence persuasion only when one or both of these conditions are not met (Chaiken, 1980 Petty and Cacioppo, 1981, 1986).Motivation to process the message and elaborate on it is largely a take of the receivers involvement. divers(prenominal) aspects of the self may be activated in a given situation, depending largely on the issue addressed, and as a result, different kinds of involvement can be ge nerated. Specifically, the message may create involvement by dealing with receivers enduring values, with receivers ability to obtain desirable outcomes or avoid undesirable outcomes, or with the impression receivers make on others Persuasive Communication Theory Page 8 (Johnson and Eagly, 1989).However, when the message has few implications for enduring values, for important outcomes, or for selfpresentation, it produces little motivation to carefully deliberate its contents. top executive to process a message is related to factors internal to the receiver as well as to external factors. Among the internal factors are familiarity with the issues and cognitive ability and intelligence, factors that tend to increase capacity for information processing and preoccupation with other matters and privation of time, which tend to reduce the ability to elaborate.External factors that increase the ability to process include message repetition and clarity of presentation, while external dis traction and use of complicated language can reduce processing ability. Some of the contextual factors studied by the Hovland group can come into play when internal or external factors lower the receivers ability to process the information presented in the message. Empirical Research When ability and motivation to process the message are low, receivers can use skirting(prenominal) cues (Petty and Cacioppo, 1986) or cognitive heuristics (Chaiken, 1980) to form their opinions.Chaiken assumed that receivers of a message, even if they are not very greatly involved, nevertheless are motivated to hold a correct view on the issue. Since, under conditions of low motivation and ability, receivers are either unable(predicate) or unwilling to deal with the merits of the advocated position, they look for contextual or encircling(prenominal) cues that might provide a basis for forming an opinion. Perhaps the most all-powerful such cue is the communicators credibility, and it may be argued th at this is the reason for the relatively consistent findings associated with communicator credibility.The heuristic strategy might in this case involve the side by side(p) line of reasoning If this expert on the matter says so, it must be right. This heuristic appears quite reasonable in that it accepts the position advocated by a credible source, even if one has not carefully scrutinized the arguments presented. Receivers can also use the sources attractiveness, or factors related to the message such as the number of arguments it contains, as peripheral cues. Thus, a message coming from a iked source might be viewed as more trustworthy, and one that contains many arguments (even if specious) might be seen as more reliable than a message that contains few arguments. Note, however, that these rules of thumb are far less convincing as a rational basis for accepting or rejecting an advocated position, and it is perhaps for this reason that factors of this kind often fail to have stro ng or consistent effects on persuasion. In any event, relying on heuristics obviates the need for careful message processing, and at the same time provides a basis for adoption of a position on the issue.Recent empirical research tends to support this view of the peripheral road to persuasion, although some complications have belatedly been noted (Johnson and Eagly, in press). Since minute reviews are available elsewhere (Chaiken, 1987 Petty and Cacioppo, 1986), we limit our discussion here to an example concerning the effects of source characteristics. Recall that communicator attractiveness was one of the source characteristics studied in the Hovland paradigm that did not have a clear and consistent effect on persuasion.If treated as a peripheral cue used only when processing motivation or ability is low, more consistent findings tend to emerge. Attractiveness of the source has been varied by attributing the message to famous versus unknown individuals (Petty, Cacioppo, and Sch umann, 1983) or to a likable versus an closed person (Chaiken, 1980). The investigators also manipulated the degree of involvement and found, as expected, that communicator attractiveness has a significantly greater effect on persuasion under low than under high involvement.Conclusions Work on the peripheral highroad to persuasion suggests that the source, message, channel, and receiver factors studied in the Hovland tradition can indeed influence the effectiveness of a message, but that this is likely to be the case only under conditions of low motivation or low ability to process the message. Such conditions can be obtained in the psychological laboratory that ensures some degree of attention by a captive audience even if the receivers have little interest in the topic or lack the ability to process the information presented (Hovland, 1959).In more naturalistic field settings, receivers who Persuasive Communication Theory Page 9 lack the motivation or ability to process a message can usually farewell the situation, while those who remain and are exposed to the message will tend to be sufficiently involved and able to process the information it contains. Persuasion by the peripheral route is clearly an inappropriate model for many realistic situations, and it is often inapplicable even in the artificial context of the laboratory.REASONING AND PERSUASION Even when it works, there is something distinctly unsatisfactory in the notification of change via the peripheral route, because the change brought about does not represent persuasion as we usually think of it. We noted at the beginning that it is the process of reasoning, the evaluation of the merits of arguments in favor and opposed to the advocated position, that is at the heart of persuasive communication. Persuasion involves more than simply oing along with an expressed point of view because of the presence of some peripheral cue it requires that the advocated position be accepted only after careful sc rutiny of the message and after lotion of whatever other information the receiver can bring to bear. Moreover, change produced by the peripheral route is generally of little practical significance. Petty and Cacioppo (1986) noted that peripheral attitude change tends to be shortlived, tends to be liable(predicate) to counterpropaganda (McGuire, 1964), and tends to have little effect on actual behavior.Clearly then, from both a theoretical and a practical point of view it would be to our advantage to focus less on the context of persuasion and more on the central processes that occur when a person is exposed to a message. Persuasion by the Central Route In the remainder of this chapter we examine persuasion that occurs when the receiver of a message is sufficiently able and motivated to give at least some scrutiny to the contents of the communication and to pass judgment the merits of the arguments it contains.This has been termed the central route to persuasion (Petty and Caciop po, 1981) and the deliberations receivers perform are known as systematic information processing (Chaiken, 1980). Instead of asking what makes a given message more effective, we must now ask how to construct an effective message. That is, what arguments, when systematically processed via the central route, will have the greatest impact on the receivers attitudes and behavior? Before we can review what is known about this question, however, we must consider the role of the receiver in greater detail. The intricacy Likelihood Model.The peripheral route to persuasion discussed earlier is one of two tracks a receiver can take in Petty and Cacioppos (1981, 1986) refinement likelihood model (ELM). The second track is persuasion via the central route. correspond to the ELM, central route persuasion depends on and is determined by the degree to which receivers elaborate on the information presented in the message. Briefly, during exposure to a persuasive communication, receivers are assu med to generate arguments of their own, either in support of the advocated position (pro arguments) or opposed to it (con arguments).These cognitive responses determine the direction and degree of change in attitudes and behavior. Increased motivation and ability to process the information in the message is, consort to the model, associated with an increase in the number of cognitive responses (pro and con arguments) generated. To the extent that the number of arguments generated on the pro side exceeds the number of arguments on the con side, the receiver will change in the advocated direction. When elaboration leads to the takings of more con than pro arguments, however, either no change or a boomerang effect (change in the opposite direction) may occur.From the communicators point of view, therefore, motivation and ability to elaborate on message content is a two-edged sword. If, on balance, the thoughts generated by the receiver favor the advocated position, then the central r oute to persuasion works to the communicators advantage. On the other hand, if the receivers cognitive responses consist preponderantly of counterarguments, then elaboration on message content can be quite detrimental to the communicators purpose.A number of studies, summarized in Petty and Cacioppo (1986), have examined the role of cognitive responses in the persuasion process. In these studies, cognitive responses are kindle Persuasive Communication Theory Page 10 in a free-response format following exposure to the message. The thoughts listed by the receivers are coded as either in favor or opposed to the advocated position, and the number of responses of each type is determined. Results, by and large, support the idea that the production of cognitive responses increases with motivation and ability to elaborate.Moreover, it is also found that changes in attitudes and behavior are consistent with the pattern of cognitive responses that are generated a balance of thoughts in favor of the advocated position tends to be associated with change in the desired direction. 2 Yielding and Impact. stipulation of cognitive responses generated by receivers in the course of exposure to the message is, however, not sufficient to account for observed changes in attitudes and behavior. For change to occur in the central mode, some of the receivers fundamental beliefs and values must undergo modification.Elaboration on the message may in fact lead to changes in cognitive structure, but evidence for the production of pro- or counter-arguments does not, in itself, assure that such changes have indeed taken place. Work on the elaboration likelihood model has focused primarily on cognitive responses to the message and has not dealt directly with changes in cognitive structure. The ideas discussed below are based on other recent work concerning persuasive communication via the central route (Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975, 1981).harmonize to Fishbein and Ajzen, a message can bring ab out changes in a receivers cognitive structure in one of two ways. First, in a process termed submissive, acceptance of arguments presented in the message can produce changes in corresponding beliefs held by the receiver. Consider, for example, a pregnant smoker who initially is not aware that cigarette smoking can adversely affect the health of her unhatched baby. This woman is now exposed to a message containing an argument and supportive evidence that establish the link between smoking and adverse health effects on the fetus.To the extent that the argument is accepted, it produces yielding in the sense that the womans cognitive structure now contains a new belief that corresponds directly to the argument in question. That is, she now believes, as stated in the message, that smoking may have ill effects on her unborn baby. Changes in a receivers chief(a) beliefs, however, can extend far beyond the information directly contained in the message. Such changes that go beyond the information given are termed impact effects.To illustrate, the pregnant woman exposed to the message that smoking can have detrimental health effects on her fetus may infer that she would feel guilty if she did not fracture smoking and that her doctor would want her to quit, even though neither argument was explicit in the message. It is also possible, however, for her to draw inferences that would work against the aims of the communicator. For example, the woman may unexpectedly form the belief that quitting would be even worse than proceed smoking because it would result in overeating.These impact effects can, of course, play a major role in the womans decision to quit or not to quit smoking. Evidence for the importance of considering yielding as well as impact effects can be found in a study on drinking reported in Ajzen and Fishbein (1980, pp. 218-242). Persuasive crinkle The challenge facing a communicator trying to produce change via the central route is to create a message that will originate approbative responses, produce yielding to its arguments, and generate impact effects in unity with the advocated change. rivalrys contained in a message can be considered effective to the extent that they influence the receivers cognitive structure. The essential question, therefore, is what makes an argument effective. In light of the fact that rhetoricians have written about argumentation for over 2,000 years, it is surprising how little empirical knowledge is available about the relative effectiveness of different types of arguments (McGuire, 1985). An analysis of this problem reveals at least three important aspects of an arguments effectiveness novelty, strength, and relevance.Below we discuss each of these aspects in turn. Argument Novelty Persuasive Communication Theory Page 11 An argument contained in a message may well be accepted (i. e. , believed to be true), but if the receiver already held the belief in question before exposure to the message, no change in belief structure would result (Fishbein and Ajzen, 1981). To be effective therefore, an argument contained in a message must not be part of the receivers initial belief system. Some empirical evidence for this marriage proposal can be found in research on group decision making (Vinokur and Burnstein, 1974).In the course of group discussions, members who offer novel arguments in support of a given decision alternative are found to be more influential than members who raise points that are well known to the rest of the group. Argument Strength Besides being novel, an argument must also be strong if it is to sway the receiver to adopt the advocated position. A strong argument is one that tends to produce agreement (positive thoughts) and does not march on generation of many counterarguments (Petty and Cacioppo, 1986). Although it is not clear what makes a strong argument, its strength or weakness can be empirically established.Earlier in this chapter we gave an illustrati ve example of a persuasive argument taken from Petty and Cacioppos (1986) research program. The argument asserted that instituting a senior comprehensive examination would raise grade point averages (see p. xx). This argument and the associated evidence make a strong case for the advocated position. Compare this to the following argument, also designed to generate support for a comprehensive exam. The National Scholarship Achievement Board recently revealed the results of a study they conducted on the effectiveness of comprehensive exams at Duke University.One major finding was that student anxiety had increased by 31%. At comparable schools without the exam, anxiety increased by only 8%. The Board reasoned that anxiety over the exams, or fear of failure, would motivate students to study more in their courses while they were taking them. It is likely that this increase in anxiety observed at Duke University would also be observed and be of benefit at other universities that adopt th e exam policy (Petty and Cacioppo, 1986, p. 57). Although this argument is quite similar in structure to the strong argument presented earlier, it appears to present a much weaker case.In fact, this argument is typically found to generate many counterarguments. Clearly, in order to create an effective message, it is in the communicators interest to spot strong arguments and avoid including arguments that tend to elicit negative thoughts about the advocated position. Argument Relevance Related to the question of an arguments strength is its relevance to the advocated position. An argument may be strong in the sense that it generates few counterarguments and many pro arguments, but if it addresses an issue that is not directly relevant to the advocated position, it may fail to produce the desired effect.This point is often not sufficiently appreciated. enjoin a communicator would like to convince students to attend an anti-apartheid demonstration in Washington, D. C. , and thus expo ses the students to a persuasive message against apartheid in South Africa. Although the arguments contained in the message may be strong in the sense that they are thinkable and generate few counterarguments, the message may not be very effective as a means of inducing students to go to Washington.To make the message more relevant in terms of this goal, one would have to include strong arguments that deal more directly with the advantages of attending the aforethought(ip) demonstration. A relevant argument, then, is one that changes those firsthand beliefs of the receiver that are directly related to the target of the influence attempt, that is, to the attitude or behavior the communicator wishes to affect. Different target variables are based on different primary beliefs, and an effective message must be tailored to fit the target in question.General discussions of different target variables and their respective foundations of primary beliefs can be found in Fishbein and Ajzen (1975, 1981) and in Fishbein and Manfredo (Chapter xx, this volume). It is beyond the scope of this chapter to provide an in-depth review. Briefly, Fishbein and Ajzen distinguish Persuasive Communication Theory Page 12 among beliefs, attitudes, intentions, and behaviors as possible targets of a persuasive communication. To effect a change in any one of these target variables, the message arguments must be directed at the primary beliefs that provide the basis for the target in question.The first step in the construction of a message, therefore, requires a decision about the relevant primary beliefs, a process that cannot be left to intuition but must be guided by a model of the targets determinants. Social psychologists have discussed a variety of approaches to understanding beliefs and attitudes and their relations to behavior, but perhaps the most popular models can be found within the framework of the theory of reasoned action (Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975 Ajzen and Fishbein, 1980) a nd its recent extension, the theory of aforethought(ip) behavior (Ajzen, 1985, 1988).The discussion below considers each target variable in turn however, a full understanding of the process is gained only by considering the relations among the different variables. Changing behavior. According to the theory of reasoned action, many behaviors of interest to social psychologists are under volitional control and, hence, are in an immediate sense determined by the intention to perform the behavior in question. A successful persuasive communication designed to change a certain behavior must therefore contain arguments that will bring about a change in the antecedent intention. The theory of planned behavior oes beyond the question of intended action, taking into account the hypothesis that the behavior of interest may not be completely under volitional control. To be successful, the message may have to provide information that will enable the receiver to gain volitional control and over come potential obstacles to performance of the behavior. A review of evidence in support of these propositions can be found in Ajzen (1988). Changing intentions. The antecedents of behavioral intentions are, according to the theory of reasoned action, the persons attitude toward the behavior and his or her infixed norm.The attitude toward the behavior refers to the evaluation of the behavior as desirable or undesirable, and the subjective norm is the perceived social pressure to perform or not to perform the behavior in question. The theory of planned behavior again adds to this model a consideration of volitional control. When issues of control arise, intentions are influenced not only by attitudes and subjective norms but also by perceived behavioral control (Ajzen and Madden, 1986 Schifter and Ajzen, 1985).A persuasive communication designed to influence intentions (and thus also behavior) can be directed at one or more of the intentions three determinants attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control. Changing attitudes. We contract at the level of primary beliefs as we consider the determinants of a persons attitudes. According to the theory of reasoned action, attitudes are a employment of salient beliefs about the attitude object (a person, group, institution, behavior or other event). Each salient belief links the object to an attribute or to an outcome in the case of a behavior.The attitude is determined by the strength of these beliefs and by the evaluations associated with the attributes (Fishbein, 1963 Ajzen and Fishbein, 1980). Beliefs about the attitude object that are salient prior to presentation of the message can be elicited in a free-response format. The message is then constructed such that it will either change some of the existent beliefs, either in their strength or their evaluations, or introduce new beliefs into the belief system. Changing beliefs. To change a specific belief on an issue, the persuasive communication has to address some of the information on which the belief is based.Several probabilistic models that link prior information to a given belief have been proposed and validated (McGuire, 1960b Wyer and Goldberg, 1970 for a review see Slovic, Fischhoff, and Lichtenstein, 1977). These models suggest that the information introduced by the persuasive communication must be information from which the belief in question can be probabilistically inferred. Conclusions The focus in recent years on the central route to persuasion holds great promise for a better understanding of persuasive communication.This route deals with the nerve centre of the persuasion process, with changes in the fundamental beliefs on which the receivers attitudes and actions are based. Although much the Great Compromiser to be done, social psychologists have gained considerable insight into some of the cognitive processes that are at work during and Persuasive Communication Theory Page 13 after exposure to a persua sive communication, and into the practical aspects of constructing an effective message. SUMMARY This chapter provided a brief historical perspective on persuasive communication theory in social psychology.No attempt was made to discuss all theoretical developments in detail as this task would require a book in itself. Instead, the focus was on a few dominant lines of theoretical development, from the beginnings of scientific research on persuasion in the 1940s to the present day. The work initiated by Hovland and his associates tended to view the receivers of a persuasive communication as passively learning the information presented and then changing their beliefs and attitudes accordingly. This view led to a concern with contextual factors, and virtual dribble of the contents of the communication and its processing by the receiver.Few generalizable conclusions emerged from the research guided by this approach, and by the late 1960s the failure of the Hovland approach was widely a cknowledged. construct was recorded when attention turned from contextual or peripheral factors to persuasion via the central route. Contextual factors were found to be important only under conditions of low involvement or low ability to process the message. It was discovered, however, as a general rule, that receivers of a message are far from passive, prosecute in an active process of analyzing and elaborating on the information presented.It became clear that the effects of a persuasive communication could not be understood unless careful attention was given to these cognitive processes. Theoretical and empirical developments of the past two decades have enabled us to consider receivers cognitive responses during exposure to a message, yielding to the arguments contained in the message, and the messages impact on other beliefs not explicitly mentioned. These developments have also resulted in a much closer examination of the contents of persuasive communications, with an eye tow ard selecting arguments that will have the maximum effect on the target of the influence attempt.In this way, the theoretical developments of recent years have important implications for the practitioner who is concerned with constructing effective persuasive communications. Persuasive Communication Theory Page 14 REFERENCES Ajzen, I. 1985. From intentions to actions A theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl &038 J. Beckmann, eds. Action-control From cognition to behavior. Heidelberg Springer 11-39. Ajzen, I. 1988. Attitudes, personality, and behavior. Chicago Dorsey Press. Ajzen, I. , &038 Fishbein, M. 1980. Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Englewood-Cliffs, NJ Prentice-Hall. Ajzen, I. &038 Madden, T. J. 1986. Prediction of purposive behavior Attitudes, intentions, and perceived behavioral control. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22 453-474. Anderson, N. H. 1971. Integration theory and attitude change. Psychological Review, 78 171-206. Asch, S. E. 1946. Forming impressions of personality. Journal of abnormal and Social Psychology, 41 258-290. Barber, T. X. 1965. Physiological effects of hypnotic suggestions a critical review of recent research. Psychological Bulletin, 4 201-222. Batra, R. , &038 Ray, M. L. 1986. Affective responses mediating acceptance of advertising.Journal of Consumer Research, 13 234-249. Boster, F. J. , &038 Mongeau, P. 1985. Feararousing persuasive messages. In R. N. Bostrom, ed. Communication Yearbook, Vol. 8. Beverly Hills, CA Sage 330-375. Brewer, W. F. 1974. There is no convincing evidence for operant or classical conditioning in adult humans. In W. B. Weimer &038 D. S. Palermo, eds. Cognition and the symbolic processes. Hillsdale, NJ Erlbaum 1-42. Bruner, J. S. 1957. On going beyond the information given. In H. E. Gruber, K. R. Hammond, &038 R. Jessor, eds. Contemporary approaches to cognition. Cambridge, MA Harvard University Press 41-69. Chaiken, S. 980. Heuristic versus systematic information pr ocessing and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39 752-766. Chaiken, S. 1987. The heuristic model of persuasion. In M. P. Zanna, J. M. Olson, &038 C. P. Herman, eds. Social Influence The Ontario Symposium, Vol. 5. Hillsdale, NJ Erlbaum 3-39. Cialdini, R. B. 1988. Influence Science and practice, 2nd Ed. Glenview, IL Scott, Foresman. Cook, T. D. , &038 Flay, B. R. 1978. The persistence of experimentally induced attitude change. In L. Berkowitz, ed. Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 1. New York Academic Press 1-57. Cronbach, L. J. 1975. Beyond the two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 30 116-127. Eagly, A. H. , &038 Himmelfarb, S. 1974. Current trends in attitude theory and research. In S. Himmelfarb &038 E. H. Eagly, eds. Readings in attitude change. New York Wiley 594610. Erdelyi, M. H. 1974. A new look at the new look perceptual defense and vigilance. Psychological Review, 81 1- 25. Fishbein, M. 1963. An investigation of the relationships between beliefs about an object and the attitude toward that object. Human Relations, 16 233-240. Fishbein, M. &038 Ajzen, I. 1975. Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA Addison-Wesley. Fishbein, M. , &038 Ajzen, I. 1981. Acceptance, yielding, and impact Cognitive processes in persuasion. In R. E. Petty, T. M. Ostrom, &038 T. C. Brock, eds. Cognitive responses in persuasion. Hillsdale, NJ Erlbaum 339-359. French, J. R. P. , &038 Raven, B. H. 1959. The bases of social power. In D. Cartwright Ed. , Studies in social power. Ann Arbor University of Michigan Press 118-149. Freedman, J. L. , &038 Fraser, S. C. 1966. Compliance without pressure The foot-inthe-door technique.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4 195-203. Higbee, K. L. 1969. Fifteen years of fear arousal Research on threat appeals 19531968. Psychological Bulletin, 72 426-444. Himmelfarb, S. , &038 Ea gly, A. H. , eds. 1974. Readings in attitude change. New York Wiley. Hovland, C. I. 1959. harmonise conflicting results derived from experimental and survey studies of attitude change. American Psychologist, 14 8-17. Hovland, C. I. , Janis, I. L. , &038 Kelley, H. H. Persuasive Communication Theory Page 15 1953. Communication and persuasion. New seaport Yale University Press. Hovland, C. I. , Lumsdaine, A. A. &038 Sheffield, F. D. 1949. Experiments on mass communication. Princeton, NJ Princeton University Press. Hovland, C. I. , &038 Weiss, W. 1951. The influence of source credibility on communication effectiveness. Public Opinion Quarterly, 15 635-650. Janis, I. L. , &038 Feshbach, S. 1953. Effects of fear-arousing communications. Journal of insane and Social Psychology, 48 7892. Johnson, B. T. , &038 Eagly, A. H. 1989. The effect of involvement on persuasion A metaanalysis. Psychological Bulletin, 106 290314. Krasner, L. 1958. Studies of the conditioning of verbal behavior. Psyc hological Bulletin, 55 148-170. Lasswell, H. D. 948. The structure and function of communication in society. In L. Bryson Ed. , Communication of ideas. New York Harper. Markiewicz, D. 1974. Effects of humor on persuasion. Sociometry, 37 407-422. McGuire, W. J. 1960a. Cognitive consistency and attitude change. Journal of irregular and Social Psychology, 60 345-353. McGuire, W. J. 1960b. A syllogistic analysis of cognitive relationships. In C. I. Hovland &038 M. J. Rosenberg, eds. Attitude organization and change. New Haven, CT Yale University Press 65-111. McGuire, W. J. 1964. Inducing resistance to persuasion Some contemporary approaches. In L. Berkowitz, ed.Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 1. New York Academic Press 191-229. McGuire, W. J. 1968. Personality and susceptibility to social influence. In E. F. Borgatta &038 W. W. Lambert, eds. Handbook of personality theory and research. Chicago Rand McNally 1130-1187. McGuire, W. J. 1969. The nature of attitudes and at titude change. In G. Lindzey &038 E. Aronson, eds. The enchiridion of social psychology, 2nd Ed. , Vol. 3. Reading, MA Addison-Wesley 136-314. McGuire, W. J. 1985. Attitudes and attitude change. In G. Lindzey &038 E. Aronson, eds. The handbook of social psychology, 3rd Ed.

No comments:

Post a Comment